Templates
for measuring the effectiveness of policies, products, and services are useful
heuristics for evaluating whether or not they contribute to the creation of “social
value.” However, there are a number of difficulties associated with the very
process of measuring social value, let alone with the establishment of a metric
capable of doing so. First, social value means different things to different
people. In many fields of social action, people don’t agree on desired outcomes.
Second, measures of social return on investment (SROI) often arbitrarily
estimate costs and paybacks [1].
A measure
of social value is only meaningful relative to the scope of the problem it
seeks to address. “Problems” and “solutions” have an inverse and mutually
reinforcing relationship. We are constantly measuring the social costs associated
with problems such as poverty, hunger, and disease. Emotional and
statistics-based appeals regarding the severity of these costs are sometimes
what spur wealthy philanthropists to back projects aimed at addressing them.
During undergrad, I used this approach while co-organizing town hall events to
discuss the disproportionate number of negative police interactions with
minority students as compared to their white peers. Our attempt at creating a
meaningful measure of the psychological trauma, academic distraction, and
emotional haranguing associated with these interactions were successful in
garnering funds from the administration for an outside investigation of campus
police. Carefully measuring the “problem” before even implementing procedural
changes allowed we as student organizers and the members of the administration
to hold one another accountable over time, and also assess the “social value”
created by the various policy changes.
If we can create metrics for the
scope of various problems to prompt action, why can’t we as easily measure the
social value generated by solutions to those problems? Perhaps it’s less about
the metrics and more about the prevailing attitudes that condition people to be
convinced of the nature of a problem by quantitative measures, but hesitant
about the legitimacy of positive social impact by similar measures. This could
be due to the negativity bias, or the notion that things of a more negative
nature have a greater effect on one’s psychological processes than do neutral
or positive things [2]. If the negativity bias has an effect on the formation
of impressions, general evaluations, decision-making, and risk considerations,
then an added element to the creation of SROIs or similar metrics of social
value would be the importance of highlighting the potential for positive social
impact given current levels of social cost. By anchoring descriptions of
supply/demand dynamics that create social value over long periods of time in
the constraints and severity levels of the related problem, social
entrepreneurs might be able to make more convincing appeals to financial
supporters. More importantly, metrics pertaining to the problem in conjunction
with supply/demand evaluations might produce more meaningful measures of social
value in the long term.
On the other hand, might thorough
evaluations of social value in relation the problems they seek to address deter
financial supporters in their risk considerations and efficiency evaluations if
the problems too large and the social value generated seems, by a comparison, a
“drop in the bucket”?
[1] Mulgan,
Geoff. (2010). Measuring Social Value. Stanford
Social Innovation Review. http://ssir.org/articles/entry/measuring_social_value
[2] Baumeister,
Roy F.; Finkenauer, Catrin; Vohs, Kathleen D. (2001). "Bad is stronger than good" Review of General Psychology (4): 323–370.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.